The Geekettez. User Research & Experience Design Studio

Get in touch

Phone Berlin: 0171.12 45 07 3
Phone Mannheim: 0177.71 38 208

From global to local: How our visual perception develops over time

6. September 2018 | User Experience, Interaction Design, UI Design

Lesedauer: 5 Minuten

“We see the forest before the trees”

Navon, D. (1977)

We live in a complex world, and the proper identification of objects is very important if we are to interact effectively with our environment. Early theories of object recognition often assumed that we first process specific single features/details of an object, followed by more general or global processing during which single features are integrated and merged.

However, there is evidence that general (or global) processing precedes specific, detailed (or local) processing. Some examples: words are recognized more quickly than individual letters, and often larger elements are perceived before the smaller ones, like in the classical Navon figure (see Fig 1).
Navon’s experiments were based on presentations of large characters which were composed of many small characters:

Fig 1: Navon Figure 1 – Ht

Source: Navon Figures from the psytoolkit demo

In the image above (Fig. 1) you see the visual stimuli used by Navon in his experiments: The letter H (global) is composed of repeated small copies of the letter T (local or feature): As you may experience the “large” letter is recognized before the smaller letters. You might also have experienced a conflict in perception.

What Navon found out was that we may favour global features—i.e., the big characters. When he asked people observing the characters to tell him if the small character in the experiment is an “H” or a “T”, people experienced a conflict in perception, which slowed down their response time. However, this conflict didn’t appear (and therefore did not slow down the response time) when he asked if the large character is an “H” or a “T.

Fig. 2: Navon Figure 2 – Ll

Source: Navon Figures from the psytoolkit demo

There was also no conflict in perception and therefore no slowdown in reaction time when both characters were the same, as shown in Fig. 2.
Thus we often see the forest (global) before the trees (local features).

However, this effect does not always occur: it can be manipulated by instructions to focus either on the so-called global (e.g the “T” in the example above or a word) or local (e.g the “S” letters in the example above or the individual characters of a word) items – or simply by placing smaller items (local items like for example letters of a word) of an object further apart or by making them bigger.

Global to local or: coarse to fine – evidence in neuroscience

This coarse (or: general)-to-fine way of visual perception is also supported by neuroscience. Several studies (Flevaris et al.,2014; Livingstone, 2000) found that visual processing develops over time – even if it seems instantaneous to us. The benefit of having such a visual system is that we quickly can perceive a whole scene but at a very low, general level – which makes total sense as it might be important in case of danger, for example.

Speaking in terms of neuroscience, there are cells in our brains which respond differently to different so-called “spatial frequencies”. Which cell type responds depends on whether we see with the cones (detail /central vision) or with the rods (peripheral vision): roughly said, it seems likely that global processing (e.g seeing a large character first) is facilitated more by a low spatial frequency which equals peripheral vision and the local processing is facilitated more by high spatial frequencies which equals more central vision (Flevaris et al.; 2014).

In the following video neurobiologist, Dr Margaret Livingstone demonstrates that a focus on these so-called spatial frequencies could help explain why the smile of the Mona Lisa is so elusive.

A screenshot from the video shows the steps respectively the time course on how we perceive objects based on the theory: very low spatial frequencies, peripheral vision (left) for the coarse or global processing, low spatial frequencies (at the centre) and high spatial frequencies and central vision (on the right) for detailed, local processing.

Fig. 3: Video Screenshot demonstrating the time course of our visual perception:

What this tells us is that neurons in our brain differ in their responsiveness to low vs high spatial frequencies.

Implications for design

Why could these findings be interesting for (interface) design?
Findings like this can help us focus on simplicity in e.g. icon design where people often will have a much harder time recognizing an icon when it is designed with too many details, which may be perceived as distracting. It takes humans longer to process all the details which can be dangerous e.g when we think of human-machine interaction in industries where people are dependent on a very fast reaction time – like in medical environments or aeroplanes.

The findings may also be important for the topic of visual hierarchy, how we arrange all elements in a design, which makes sure that people can easily find the information they are looking for and which separates important from not-so-important information and visual distractors and clutter.

If your goal is a speedy perception of your design (which is very important in specific areas of human-computer interaction) we should keep the time course of visual perception in mind when designing because this will help us improve that design by creating (or using) elements that are fast to recognize or that people are already familiar with.

Further reading

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383.
  • Online demo for the Navon task: https://www.psytoolkit.org/experiment-library/navon.html
  • Hegdé, J. (2008). Time course of visual perception: Coarse-to-fine processing and beyond. Progress in Neurobiology, 84, 405–439.
  • Flevaris, A.V., Martinez, A. & Hillyard, S.A. (2014). Attending to global versus local stimulus features modulates neural processing of low versus high spatial frequencies: An analysis with event- related brain potentials. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 (Article 277).

Ähnliche Beiträge